CONCISE HISTORY
OF THE
LANGUAGE SCIENCES
FROM THE SUMERIANS TO THE COGNITIVISTS
SECTION X
20th Century Linguistics
Trends in Twentieth-Century Linguistics: An Overview
1. ‘Progress’ and ‘Science’ in Linguistics
In order to evaluate and understand the twentieth
century with any sort of objectivity, we need to extend
our perspective backward in time. Historical continuities
with the nineteenth and earlier centuries give
a firmer grounding to judgments concerning the twentieth
century. For linguistics, such continuities are
manifest:
the founders of twentieth-century linguistic
theory, trained in that of the late nineteenth, rejected
few of its fundamental principles but sought instead
to extend their domain. Looking back over the whole
of the past 200 years, a still grander continuity
emerges, one that sweeps over and above the paradigmatic
and methodological breaks and splinters that
occupy our attention most of the time. It is the gradual
realignment of the study of language away from moral
science, philosophy, aesthetics, rhetoric, and philology,
and in the direction of the natural sciences-
first botany, biology, chemistry, and comparative
anatomy; then geology; and finally physics, by way of
mathematics. With this has come a steady elimination
of.human will from the object of study, the necessary
condition for any ‘science’ in the modern sense. Over
these same two centuries science has become virtually
synonymous with academic prestige, as measured by
institutionalization (creation of departments and pos-
itions, launching of journals, organizing of conferences),
financial support from governmental and other
grant-giving
agencies, and public recognition. Not
surprisingly, then, for linguists progress came to be
equated with scientificization. ’
When in the first part of the twentieth century the
great achievements of the nineteenth-century fore-
bears were stated, it was in terms of the new methodological
rigor they introduced into the analysis of
language,
and of their success in abandoning formerly
connected fields, such as philology and mythologyrather
than, say, how many more languages and
linguistic phenomena were described and accounted
for than previously, or how far language teaching had
advanced. In other words, progress was defined as the
John E. Joseph
acquisition of autonomous status for linguistic science
(see Sect. 3.6). As for the mid-twentieth-century
rapprochement with mathematics and physics, it was
carried out in full consciousness and with overt
references; ‘mathematical linguistics* (now usually
called computational linguistics) would even emerge
as a significant subdiscipline.
2. Linguistic ‘Mainstreams’
These developments did not affect all work on language
equally. Rather, the ‘mainstream’ was pro-
gressively redefined in the direction of autonomous
inquiry. By mainstream is meant the group having
the greatest institutional power and prestige, with no
implication that other approaches were ‘backwater’
in any sense other than the hold they possessed on the
major journals, support agencies, academic departments,
and public attention. Areas the mainstream
deserted
continued to interest other linguists, some-
times even to inspire new disciplines like general semantics,
philosophy of language, and critical theory, to
name
just three. But in effect, ‘linguistics’ has come to
designate a more or’less autonomous approach to
language,
and this brief overview will not depart
significantly from the traditional mainstream focus.
Defining linguistics was in fact the main goal of the
book generally credited with shaping the twentieth-
century linguistic agenda: the Cours de linguistique
gPnPrafe of Ferdinand de Saussure (1857-1913). As
discussed in Sect. 4, Saussure established the framework
for the ‘synchronic’ (nonhistorical) study of the
language
system, or lungue,
conceived as the socially-
shared system of signs deposited in the mind of each
speaker. Nevertheless, during at least the first third of
the twentieth century the study of language continued
to be dominated by the historical inquiry which had
earned such prestige in the nineteenth century. Only
gradually did synchronic linguistics gain practitioners
and institutional acceptance, until eventually his-
torical linguistics was itself partially marginalized and
fundamentally refashioned in the light of synchronic
findings.
221
20th Centurv Linguistics
From the 1930s through the 1950s the mainstream
of linguistics was defined by various American and
European ‘schools’ (understood as groups of linguists
sharing some basic common assumptions about prob-
lems and methodology, while often disagreeing on
particular matters) which are today grouped together
as ‘structuralist’ (see Sect. 5). All of them had some
greater or lesser intellectual debt to Saussure’s Cours
and to the groundwork laid by historical-comparative
study. From the 1960s to the present, the mainstream
has been defined by the ‘generativist’ approaches
which originated in the work of Noam Chomsky (b.
1928; see Sect. 8). But as the twentieth century comes
to a close, the synthesis of the last 30 years appears to
be dissolving. Linguistics has splintered into a panoply
of well-entrenched approaches that are roughly equal
in prestige, and the field as a whole is coming under the
shadow of emerging megadisciplines like cognitivism
and connectionism (see Sect. 12).
3. Language Theory before World War I
By 1900 the firm hold which historical grammar had
held upon mainstream status in linguistic science was
being challenged by several adjacent fields of study.
Even within the historical sphere linguists did not
agree which if any of the leads provided by various
versions of psychology should be followed. This sec-
tion surveys what part of the general linguistics territory
each field claimed as its own.
3.1 Historical Linguistics
By 1890 the mainstream of the field was definitively
occupied by the approach which had been established
around 1876 by the Junggrammatiker (Neogrammarians)
of the University of Leipzig, whose work
followed up on that of August Schleicher ( 1821-l 868).
It excluded virtually all manifestations of language
except historical phonology, morphology, and syntax
(in descending order of attention), and was primarily
concerned with the Indo-European family and par-
ticular subgroupings within it. Phonology and morphology
covered that part of language that could be
cataloged as positive facts; syntax, on the other hand,
had to be stated in relational terms, and for most
known languages it involved a considerable volitional
factor. Schleicher had excluded syntax from linguistic
‘science’ on the grounds that it was subject to free
will. Although syntax continued to be of marginal
importance relative to phonology, some important
work in this area was carried out, notably by Berthold
Delbruck (1842-1922).
By focusing their inquiry in this way the neogram-
marians succeeded brilliantly in meeting the criteria
for progress of their time. It seemed to many that they
had done virtually all that it was possible for a true
science of language (defined according to the then
dominant ideology of positivism) to do. This is the
impression one takes away from the first major his-
222
toriographical study of linguistics in the modern
period, Holger Pedersen’s (1867-1953) Linguistic Sci-
ence in the 19th Centuq* (193 1). But this progress was
gained at the price of ignoring ‘general’ linguistic
theory and leaving most aspects of language to the
inquiry of adjacent fields. In particular, psychology
annexed most aspects of language production and
comprehension early on, a move hastened by the enor-
mous influence of Wilhelm von Humboldt’s (17671835)
posthumously published treatise on language
structure
and mental development (1836). Even at the
height of the neogrammarian ascendance, dissenting
voices could be heard within the historical domain-
most notably that of Hugo Schuchardt (1842-1927),
who launched a major attack against the Leipzig
mainstream in 1885.
3.2 Psycholog?
‘Classical’ psychology of the mid-nineteenth century
was the very antithesis of positivism, formulating the-
ories of mind and thought in a mode that we would
today classify as philosophical. Psychological linguists
in the Humboldtian tradition like Heymann Steinthal
(1823-99) saw their investigations of language as a
means to the understanding of national culture and
thought. In particular, their work on the typology of
languages continued to explore the parallels between
mental structures and morphosyntactic structures.
This mode did not disappear even when, a generation
later, ‘experimental’ psychologists incorporated enough
positivist methodology into their practice to
maintain its scientific status and prestige. One of the
most prominent figures of this period, Wilhelm Wundt
( 1832-1920), developed a V&lkerpsychologie (psychology
of nations) with a specifically linguistic component,
and it gained enormous prestige, informing
for example the first book by the great American
linguist Leonard Bloomfield (see Sect. 5.2). But other
linguists continued to object to the fact that the
psychological approach worked backward from
a priori notions about the nature and structure of the
mind to form theories of language that could never be
empirical or objective in anything but a superficial
sense.
3.3 Phonetics and Dialectology
Experimental phonetics, the detailed measurement of
speech sounds, offered the first truly positivistic
approach to language, and had steadily grown in prestige
through the influence of such individuals as Alex-
ander Melville Bell (1819-1905) and Henry Sweet
(1845-1912). Objective and quantitative as it was, no
one could dispute its claims to scientificness. But while
its descriptive power was unparalleled, and its peda-
gogical usefulness high, its explanatory power proved
disappointing, especially to those who believed pho-
netic principles would provide the explanations for
historical change. Phonetics could only deal with
individual speech acts, not the abstract linguistic systems
that underlay them, and its data tended to form
an end in themselves. Whereas ‘classical’ psychology
had suffered from being too cerebral and not empirical
enough, phonetics was so single-mindedly empirical
as to defy rational interpretation.
Still, in the age of positivism phonetics opened the
possibility of accumulating masses of previously
untapped data about living dialects. Detailed research
on German dialects by Georg Wenker (1852-l 9 11) in
the 1870s began a trend that would reach maturity
with the IO-volume linguistique de la France
(1902-10) by the Swiss linguist Jules Gillieron (1854
1926) and his assistant Edmond Edmont (1849-1926).
Led by Gillieron, the linguistic geographers developed
their own critique of neogrammarian theory: against
the dictum of absolute sound laws modulated only by
analogy, Gillieron stressed the impact of factors like
homophonic clashes and popular etymology. This led
to the resuscitation of a doctrine originally attributed
to Jacob Grimm (1785-l 863), which became the rallying
cry of the anti-neogrammarian resistance: ‘every
word
has its own history.’
3.4 Anthropology
Late in the nineteenth century, as anthropology
moved from a physical toward a cultural orientation,
an impressive fieldwork methodology developed
based on positivistic principles. Since language was
taken to be an integral element of culture, and since
linguists were concerned mostly with tracing the history
of Indo-European tongues, anthropologists had
little choice but to undertake the description of
unknown languages on their own. Franz Boas (1859-
1942), a German emigre to America, became the
organizational leader of anthropological linguistics
and began a tradition of ‘scientific’ description of living
languages within their own cultural framework,
free of preformed ideas, including those of the psychologists.
This is not to say that Boas ever rejected
. psychological concepts from anything but the collection
and analysis of language data; nor did he reject
the historical approach, since much of his activity
‘ was aimed at establishing the historical affiliations of
’ American Indian tribes through their linguistic
relations. Some have even seen a trace of Hum-
+ boldtian linguistic thinking in the emphasis Boas
I
’ placed on diversity over and above communality. In
any case, Boas’s school was probably the closest thing
! to a meeting ground for the various approaches to
;’ language at the start of the century, and as we shall see,
Y it would take a leading role in American structuralism
{: with the work of Boas’s student and associate Edward
” Sapir (see Sect. 5.2).
z
I 4.:
f’ 3.5 Sociology
F The young science of sociology also embodied the
spirit of positivism, with which it shared the same rec-
;
‘,
c c
6
?
ik.
5.
4
:r
’ Trends in Twentieth-century Linguistics: An Overview
ognized founder, Auguste Comte (1798-l 857), and as
the new century opened it had begun to seize a con-
siderable portion of the intellectual territory once
claimed by classical psychology, which by now ap-
peared hopelessly old-fashioned and metaphysical.
Emile Durkheim (1858-1917), who had gone to
Germany for postgraduate study under Wundt but
found his approach less than satisfactory, assumed the
first chair in social science at Bordeaux in 1896 and
obtained a professorship in Paris in 1902. Also in 1896
he founded the periodical L ‘Annie sociologique, whose
principal linguistic contributor would be Saussure’s
student and close associate Antoine Meillet (18661936).
However, until the
Cours, anything like a sociological
formulation of linguistics would remain a
vague desideratum. Wundt’s national psychology still
claimed this aspect of language for its own.
3.4 To\i’ard an Autonomous General Linguistics
The roughly coeval rise to prominence of Boas’s
anthropology in the United States, Gillieron’s dialect
geography and Durkheim’s sociology in France,
Sweet’s articulatory phonetics in the United King-
dom, and Wundt’s national psychology in Germany
conspired to give a new impetus to the study of living
languages that mainstream linguistics had long since
abandoned. Not that all historical linguists had ever
been content with the division of labor outlined above:
some thought that historical-comparative linguistics
alone could be scientific, others felt that other aspects
could be studied scientifically but that this should fall
to adjacent, disciplines, and still others thought that
historical-comparative linguistics should be expanded
to take the other areas under its wing.
The last group faced the double disadvantage of
having to emphasize the failures of nineteenth-century
linguistic science and of offering potential students
and the general public few clues as to how to reach
the goals they set for themselves. Linguists with a
basically historical orientation who published notable
books on general linguistics in the late nineteenth and
early twentieth century include William Dwight Whitney
(1827-94),
Michel Brtal (1832-1915), Abel
Hovelacque ( 1843-96), Archibald Henry Sayce (18451933),
Victor Henry (1850-1907), and Hanns Oertel
(1868-1952). The works of Georg von der Gabelentz
(1840-93) and especially Hermann Paul (1846-192 1)
deserve particular mention because although cast in
the historical mold they anticipate the vision of the
linguistic system that would characterize the struc-
turalist period. But it was Whitney, first and foremost,
who showed the way toward a modern general lin-
guistics that would not be a smorgasbord where psychology,
phonetics, and other subspecialties were
served
in equal portions, but a comprehensive study
of language guided by historical principles and examining
language for its own sake-a truly ‘autonomous’
approach.
223
Century* Linguistics
One other prominent contributor to genera! linguistics
needs to be discussed here: Otto Jespersen (1860-
1943). Jespersen, who gained his early renown in phonetics
and the history of English, undertook in the
1920s an attempt to delineate the ‘logic’ of grammar
divorced from psychological underpinnings-work
that among other things anticipates future directions
in its attention to syntax and child language acquisition,
Yet Jespersen would expressly reject some of
the
key tenets of Saussure’s Co~lrs
and structuralism,
making him the last great general linguist in the prestructuralist
vein.
4. Saussure and the Cows
The decisive step in redirecting the linguistic mainstream
to the study of living languages was taken by
Ferdinand de Saussure (1857-1913) in three courses
on general linguistics he gave at the University of
Geneva between 1907 and 1911, a synthesis of which
was published posthumously as the Cows de lin-
guistique gknkrale (Course in General Linguistics) in
19 16 (see the article on Saussurean Tradirion in
Linguistics). Inspired in part by Whitney’s views on
the special fitness of linguists to direct the study of
languages, living or dead, Saussure problematized
these issues in a clearer and more methodical fashion
than anyone before him. He maintained that language
as an abstract system (langue) was the proper object
of study of linguistics, and that ‘synchronic’ study of
language as a static system should be kept rigorously
distinct from ‘diachronic’ (i.e., historical, langue-
focused) study.
Saussure delineated for the first time a program that
would be neither historical nor ahistorical, neither
psychological nor apsychological; yet more systematic
than Whitneyan or Paulean general linguistics, so as to
compare favorably in intellectual and methodological
rigor with the rival approaches outlined in Sect. 3.
Saussure tended increasingly toward sociological
rather than psychological formulations of langue over
the years in which he lectured on general linguistics.
Again, this may be tied in part to the need to establish
synchronic linguistics independently of either the
dominant psychological establishment (Wundt) or the
neogrammarians, who had combined with the psychologists
to greater or lesser degrees. As suggested in
Sect. 3.5, the young science of sociology offered one
of the most progressive approaches to human phenomena,
and was not yet so well-instituted as to pose
any threat to the emergence of an autonomous general
linguistics.
But most importantly, Saussure’s program surpassed
all rival approaches in crucial aspects of scien-
tificization. The marginalization of actual speech
production (parole) provided a quantum leap toward
the elimination of volitional factors; the abstract lan-
guage system, langue, is beyond the direct reach of the
individual will. Secondly, Saussure’s characterization
224
of langue as a system of ‘arbitrary* relations between
spoken ‘signifiers’ (i.e., sound patterns) and mental
‘signifieds’ (i.e., concepts)--relations that are of pure
form, where elements may in effect have any substance
so long as they differ from one another-moved
linguistics away from its nineteenth-century con-
nections with biology (a science largely pass4 in academic
glamour) and in the direction of mechanical
physics, the mathematically-directed study of the
physical universe, which was reascending to the forefront
of scientific prestige after years of relative neglect.
Like its predecessors, Saussure’s program brought
progress as much through what it excluded as what it
added. Although he spoke of a linguistics of parole
that would cover the phonetic side of language and
the products of individual will, he made it clear that
investigating larlgue is the essential, real linguistics.
Similarly, his program for diachronic linguistics was
meant to reform, not marginalize, the historical study
of language, yet such was the impact of his synchronic
program that it dealt historical linguistics a blow from
which it has never fully recovered. In both instances
the Cows became the touchstone for developments
that were probably inevitable, given the overall pres-
sures for the rise of an autonomous science of living
languages and the general evolution of academic pres-
tige toward mathematical and physical approaches.
5. The Emergence of Structuralist Schools
The end of World War I (1914-18) brought a widespread
sense of liberation from a century of German
linguistic dominance. Linguists outside Germany,
while still respectful of the neogrammarians’ methods,
now felt free to use, correct, or abandon them as they
saw fit. In the first decade of the twentieth century the
formulation of a national linguistics had meant the
application of neogrammarian techniques to the study
of German dialects, and even opposition views had to
be defined relative to the Leipzig mainstream. But
from the 1920s on a national linguistics came to mean
a more or less original theoretical position held by a
nation’s leading linguists. Clearly, the postwar gen-
eration was ready for change.
In any survey of early structuralism the Geneva
School deserves pride of place, for the role of Charles
Bally (1865-l 947) and Albert Sechehaye (1870-l 946)
in publishing the Cours and of Serge Karcevskij (see
Sect. 5.3) in transmitting Saussure’s doctrines to Mos-
cow and Prague, as well as for the important original
work done by these and other members (see The
Geneva School of Linguistics after Saussure). Yet the
Geneva School would be largely overshadowed by
developments in other quarters, the most significant
of which are surveyed in Sects. 5.24.
5.1 Features of Structuraiism
The term ‘structuralism’ (which did not come into use
in linguistics until the late 1920s) indicates a number
of approaches to the study of language which arose
at this time, having in common the following features:
(a)
W
@>
(4
(d
As
The study of ‘systematic’ phenomena more or
less along the lines of Saussure’s charac-
terization of langue. (It has been noted that
even Bally, in attempting to realize a linguistics
of parole in his ‘stylistics,’ ended up by incorporating
stylistic phenomena into the sphere of
langue.)
In conjunction with (a), an implied belief that
‘abstract’ levels of analysis are more funda-
mental, more deep-seated-in a word, more
‘real’-than concrete ones.
A preference for ‘social’ abstractions over mental
ones, including an axiomatic faith in lan-
guage as a fundamentally social phenomenon
which nevertheless could best be studied
through the utterances of individual speakers.
In conjunction with (c), a general priority of
linguistic ‘form’ over meaning-though see
Sect. 5.4 on the ‘London School.’ (This is a
continuing heritage from the neogrammarians,
whose single-minded concentration on form
had inspired Brial to bring forth ‘semantics’ in
reaction.)
A deep distrust in written language, which is
usually characterized as not being language at
all but only a secondary representation-
though see Sect. 5.3 on the Prague School. This
feature seems however to be on a distinct level
from the other four: contingent rather than
necessary to the structuralist outlook, and cer-
tainly not restricted to it.
will be clear from the following survey, struc-
turalist linguistics arose across Europe and America
not in a unified fashion, but in the form of national
schools-not (contrary to a long-standing mythj
through lack of contact, but because of a desire for
.: intellectual independence (especially after the decades
? of German domination) and for theories that would
i: reflect the different linguistic interests and ideologies
:, of the various countries. Yet the postwar generation
*’ all sought approaches that appeared modern and
$ scientific, and they landed on largely the same things.
it” The Cours was a major influence on all the struc-
x,:, turalist schools, though by no means the only one; it
k: ,provided a theoretical program but little in the way
k+ of actual work to be carried out. All in all, the struc!$.turalist
period is surprising both in its unity and its
i, diversity.
+.
;* 6..
‘>-
.,$.2, American Structuralism
,The two most prominent American linguists of the
first half of the twentieth century, Edward Sapir
‘(1884-1939) and Leonard Bloomfield (1887-1949) folIowed
parallel and convergent career paths (see Amer-
in Twentieth-century Linguistics: An Overview
ican Srructuralism). Both were active, together with
Boas and others, in the institutionalization of linguis-
tics in America and in developing and refining the
analytical method known as ‘distributional’ because
of its classification of elements according to the
environments in which they appear. Yet where Sapir’s
ideas are embedded in (though never subordinated to)
a broad cultural-anthropological perspective, Bloomfield
(formerly an adherent to Wundt’s Vdkerpsy-
chologie) had become a behaviorist, and treated
languages as systems of stimuli and responses. Meaning,
being unavoidably mentalistic, was suspect to
Bloomfield, unless it was determined objectively on
the basis of distribution. (Some of Bloomfield’s stu-
dents and followers would develop a still more radical
position, virtually exiling meaning from the purview
of linguistics altogether, though it is a mistake to
associate this position with Bloomfield himself.)
Despite their general convergence, then, Bloomfield’s
view was more narrowly linguistic than Sapir’s
and profited from its attachment to the empirical and
‘modern’ science of behaviorism. Such was the success
of Bloomfield’s 1933 book Language that it effectively
set the agenda of American linguistics for a generation
to come. Sapir and his students contributed at least
as much as Bloomfield and the (neo-)Bloomfieldians
to the refinement of the distributional method and
phonemic theory, but never forsook their broader
anthropological interests. Sapir’s student Benjamin
Lee Whorf (1897-194 1) pursued a line of inquiry into
the notion that the structure of thought might be
dependent upon the structure of the linguistic system.
This idea, later dubbed the ‘Sapir-Whorf hypothesis*,
has roots dating back at least as far as John Locke
(1632-1704). In a sense it is the ultimate expression of
faith in the power of the linguistic system; but in any
case it was anathema to the anti-mentalist Bloom-
fieldians, and even today it continues to arouse controversy.
5.3 The Prague School
Despite important contributions by its founder VilCm
Mathesius (1882-1945) and other Czech members,
the Prague Linguistic Circle (founded 1926) is best
remembered for the work of three prominent Russi-
ans, Roman Jakobson (18961982), Serge Karcevskij
(1884-1955), and N. S. Trubetzkoy (1890-1938) (see
also Prague School Phonology). Jakobson had been a
prominent member of the Moscow Linguistic Circle,
a center of the Russian formalist movement, in which
certain of the features listed in Sect. 5. l-most notably
the priority of form over meaning-had arisen inde-
pendently from Saussure (an indication that they were
inherent in the Zeitgeist). Karcevskij had been at
Geneva from 1906 to 1917, years that span Saussure’s
courses in general linguistics, and when he returned
to Moscow after the October Revolution in 1917 he
225
20th Century Linguistics
brought back a first-hand familiarity with Saussurean
thought. Jakobson and Trubetzkoy recognized the
points of convergence with formalism and earlier
work by Russian linguists, but also appreciated the
originality of Saussure‘s systematization.
The ‘Theses Presented to the First Congress of
Slavic Philologists in Prague, 1929’ already evince the
distinctive characteristics of Prague structuralism,
namely breadth-they include programs for the study
of poetic language and applications to language teach-
ing-and ‘functionalism.’ The document begins: ‘Language
like any other human activity is goal-oriented’
(Steiner
1982: 5). Besides any immediate material goal
to be accomplished, Prague inquiry assumed a constant,
implicit goal of maximally efficient communi-
cation, whether in the case of a casual utterance or
some manifestation of poeticity. The Prague School
also devoted considerable attention to analyzing the
special nature of’standard languages,’ a topic in which
they had a very practical interest given the need to
establish and maintain a national language acceptable
to both Czechs and Slovaks that had existed since the
creation of Czechoslovakia in 1918.
In the 1930s Jakobson and Trubetzkoy took structuralism
in the radically new direction of what is now
called ‘markedness’ theory, which holds that certain
elements in the linguistic system have an inter-
relationship that is neither arbitrary nor purely
formal, but defined by the fact that one element is
distinguished from the other through the addition of
an extra feature, a ‘mark.’ When the distinction is
neutralized it is always the simple, ‘unmarked’ member
of the opposition that appears. This concept,
which undoes the strict separation of substance and
form, first arose in Trubetzkoy’s phonology studies;
Jakobson then extended it to morphology and other
structural levels, ultimately developing it into a theory
of linguistic ‘naturalness’ in which unmarked elements
are predicted to be those which occur most widely
-across languages, are acquired first in childhood, and
are lost last in aphasia.
Following his emigration to America in 1942,
Jakobson exercised a fundamental impact on the
development of structuralism, both through his con-
ceptual innovations and his success in exporting his
brand of structuralism to (a) other human and natural
sciences, where it became the dominant paradigm in
the 1950s and 1960s and (b) American linguistics,
through his influence (both direct and indirect) on
Chomsky (see Sect. 8). Besides America, much of the
later history of Prague structuralism was played out
in Paris, in the work of Andre Martinet (b.1908)
whose ‘Functional Linguistics’ continues to this day
to develop key aspects of the Prague program (see
Grummur). But it is particularly with Jakobson
and his followers, including Meillet’s student Emile
Benveniste (1902-76), that the structuralist concept of
the system is elaborated to near-metaphysical pro-
226
portions, while opening a vein of insights that linguists
of many schools continue to mine.
5.4 Olher Sfructuralist Currents
Two other traditions have been particularly influential,
though neither so much as those of America or
Prague. The first is the ‘Copenhagen School’ headed
by Louis Hjelmslev (1899-1965), who went farther
than any of his contemporaries toward working out
the ‘relational’ nature of linguistic systems as implied
in Saussure’s Cows (see The Glossematic School of
Linguisrics). In principle the concern with form over
substance was a common structuralist heritage, but as
carried to its logical extreme by Hjelmslev it resulted
in a higher degree of abstraction than Prague functionalism
or American distributionalism could tolerate.
Hjelmslev anticipated the ‘algebraic’ quality of
post-World War II linguistics, and it is indeed in this
later period that his primary influence is felt (see Sect.
7).
The other important structuralist tradition, the
‘London School’ founded by John Rupert Firth
(1890-1960), deviates from the other schools in its
treatment of feature (d), the priority of form over
meaning, a fact which differentiates it most sharply
from the Bloomfieldians (see Firth and Ihe London
School of Linguisrics). In fact, Firth approached the
whole systematic nature of language in an unparalleled
way. Whereas other schools conceived of language
systems as consisting of a small set of largely
independent subsystems (phonology, morphology,
syntax, suprasegmentals), for Firth language was
‘polysystemic, ’ incorporating an infinite number of
interdependent micro-systems which overlap the tra-
ditional levels of analysis. The London School’s
refusal to separate phonology and suprasegmentals,
for example, made interaction with American structuralists
almost impossible-yet it anticipated work
in generative phonology by nearly half a century (see
Sect. 8). The ‘neo-Firthian’ systemic linguistics of M.
A. K. Halliday (b-1925; see Systemic Theory) and his
followers represents, together with tagmemics (Sect.
7; see also Tugmemics), one of the most robust ongoing
continuations of an essentially structuralist
.
Finally, special mention is due to Meillet’s protege
tradition.
Gustave Guillaume (I 883-1960), a relatively isolated
figure on the Parisian scene who cut his own structuralist
path distinct from the dominant Pragueanism
of Martinet and (in a different vein) Benveniste (see
Guilluumean Linguistics). Like Hjelmslev, Guillaume
was largely concerned with elaborating the systematic
and abstract program of Saussure’s Cours, but less
algebraically and with more concern for linguistic data
and psychological mechanisms. Guillaume’s work was
centered on French syntax, with a special predilection
for analysis of the definite and indefinite article, which
(in French at least) stands on the border between
,
svntax and semantics. Here he was clearly ahead of
$s time, and it may be no surprise that hehas gained
his widest audience only during the 1970s and
1980s.
6, Developments in Historical Linguistics
Many linguists interpreted Saussure‘s arguments for
synchronic study as implying that it alone was true
linguistics-despite the fact that half of the Cours is
devoted to diachronic matters. Jakobson took the lead
in insisting that each of the two approaches actually
implies the other. Certainly of the five features of
structuralism in Sect. 5.1, none is blocked a priori
from application to the diachronic dimension; and
none of those linguists at the forefront of structuralism
ever shied from tackling historical problems. Indeed,
Jerzy Kurylowicz’s (1895-1978) 1927 demonstration
that the distribution of the letter in the recently
identified and transcribed ancient language Hittite
corresponded precisely to that of the abstract and
hypothetical ‘sonant coefficients’ posited for ProtoIndo-European
by Saussure in 1878, did much to con-
vince historical linguists of the value of structural
inquiry, and helped remind structuralists of the valu-
able corroboration historical data could provide to
their theories.
Yet during the same period historical linguistics
became the locus of a reaction against not just struc-
turalism but the whole scientificization that had been
underway for over a century. The point of departure
for this reaction was Benedetto Croce’s (1866-1952)
call in 1900 for the return of language study to the
realm of moral science and the human will. In view of
the development of the field as outlined in Sect. 1, it
is not surprising either that most mainstream linguists
ignored Croce or that certain individuals (particularly
in Italy) took up his call with great fervor. Karl
Vossler’s (1872-1949) attempt at a linguistic appli-,
cation of Croce’s theories attracted a wide following,
especially in German-speaking lands. The mainstream
linguist most deeply affected was Matte0 Bartoli
(1873-1946). the founder of ‘neolinguistics’ (later
: called ‘area1 linguistics’), an approach to historical
>:
study combining Crocean ideas with the findings of
id7 dialect geography to create a counterpoint to neo-
‘, grammarianism (see Neolinguisric School in Italy).
i After the deaths of Bartoli and Vossler the movement
I’
3
faded out, though not before the tenets of area1
? linguistics became well established in the historical
$5 and geographical approaches.
?;
a
Another unique development arose in the former
i.. Soviet Union with the work of Nikolai Jakovlevich
c
p
Marr (1865-1934), who argued for the existence of a
@and historical macro-family of languages he called
r; ‘Noetic,‘ whose principal branches would be Semitic
k
::.
and ‘Japhetic.’ The latter family, another of his inventions,
included originally his native Georgian and the
plausibly related Basque, but grew over the decades
Trends in Twentieth-century Linguistics: An Overview
to subsume many extinct languages of the Mediterranean
region (e.g., Iberian, Etruscan, Elamite)
and was ultimately extended far into Asia, Africa,
northern Europe (including eventually the Celtic and
Germanic languages), and even America. Marr
believed that this original Japhetic cultural unity-
classless, communistic, and not tied to race-fell victim
to ‘Aryan’ conquerors whose descendants would
spawn
European capitalism and imperialism. He
insisted
that suppressing this history was the goal of
‘bourgeois linguistics,’ i.e., the Western European historical-comparative
tradition.
Marr’s ‘New Theory of Language’ asserted that
language was a superstructural element in Marxist
terms (i.e., a direct consequence of the economic and
social system), and strongly contested the Western
European view of linguistic history as proceeding
from unity to diversity, arguing that because of continuous
language mixture we actually move in the
opposite
direction. Marr’s ‘paleontological analysis*
reduced all the words of all languages to four basic
elements-.&, her, van, and rosh, occurring singly or
in combination-and analyzed their subsequent his-
tory according to his theory of ‘stadialism,‘ which
held that economic revolution must produce linguistic
evolution.
Outside the USSR, Marr’s theories were dismissed
as methodologically unsound and ideologically
driven; his fantastic etymologies and genealogies and
his claims to penetrate the thoughts and mental development
of prehistoric peoples were beyond the pale
of scholarly objectivity. The strongest repudiation of
his views came in 1950 when his own countrymen, led
by Stalin himself, denied that language was superstructural,
dismissed Japhetic theory and paleontological
analysis,
and embraced the historical-
comparative method as scientifically sound.
The 1980s saw a revival of interest in linguistic
‘mega-families’ like Nostratic (embracing at least
Indo-European, Semitic, Georgian and Basque) and
the vast American Indian reconstructions of Joseph
Greenberg (see Sect. 10). These proposals, while at
first attracting enormous attention in the popular
press, were met with skepticism from the ranks of
historical linguists and have largely retreated into specialized
corners.
7. Post-World War II ‘Algebraic’ Structuralism
From about 1945 younger linguists showed an
increasing bent toward the algebraic and mathematical
aspects of structuralism, in the use of tables,
formulas, and other mathematical schemata, statistics,
calculations, and the generally rigorous working
out of the notions of system and systematicity. Such
a bent had already figured prominently in the work of
Hjelmslev and Guillaume (Sect. 5.4). In the early
1950s military and commercial interest in furthering
the wartime progress on computers and machine
227
b@
.
translation improved the fortunes of many linguists,
particularly in America, and gaye even more impetus
to the development of computationally-based models.
In America, the ‘neo-Bloomfieldians’ assumed the
mainstream mantle they had previously shared with
the disciples of Sapir (see American Structuralism),
and ‘anthropological linguistics’ retreated to the
status of a subdiscipline. Bloomfield’s mathematically
inclined heir apparent Charles F. Hackett (b.1916)
rose to prominence, as did Zellig S. Harris (1909-92),
whose Methods of Structural Linguistics (completed
1947, published 1951) marked the high point in the
systematization of Bloomfieldian analysis. Harris,
Hackett, and Jakobson also began extending their
inquiry to syntax, a largely neglected area (despite a
number of high-quality contributions over the years,
especially in the historical domain). Although syntactic
studies would not come fully into their own
until
the ascendence
of Chomsky, who declared a
sharp break with the structuralist (especially the neoBloomfieldian)
tradition (Sect. 8),
nevertheless in his
wake further structuralist accounts of syntax were put
forward, of which the most notable are the ‘stratificational
grammar’ of Sydney M. Lamb (b. 1929),
which follows largely in the tradition of Hjelmslev (see
Stratifjcational Grammar: The Giossematic School of
Linguistics), and the ‘tagmemics’ of Kenneth L. Pike
(b.1912; see Tagmemics).
Pike, who had taken courses with Sapir and Bloomfield,
has had enormous influence not only for his
definitive
work on phonemic theory but also for his
decades-long
association with the Summer Institute
of Linguistics and the Wycliffe Bible Translators,
dedicated to translating the gospel into every one of
the world’s languages. Through their auspices hundreds
if not thousands of descriptive linguists have
been
trained in tagmemic analysis, and as a result, our
knowledge
of the structure of many languages exists
only
in this form. The religious side of Pike’s activity
has caused it to be neglected in accounts ‘of modern
linguistics, but it is largely the reason why the structuralist
tradition continues at present to have a significant
existence throughout the world.
In Europe too syntactic studies were underway, following
on the pioneering work of Lucien Tesniere
(1893-1954;
see Valency
Grammar), but the focus of
structuralist investigation continued to be phonology,
and dialect geographic and historical linguistics con-
tinued to be more actively pursued than in the USA.
Meanwhile the younger generation of European
scholars looked increasingly to America for innovative
ideas and technological advances. Hence the
major development in structuralism during this period
was its exportation to other fields-until a revolt
against structuralism became part of the student upris-
ings of 1968. From the mid-1960s to the mid-1970s
European linguistics turned increasingly toward
American generativism, while the other human sciences
played out a ‘poststructuralist’ phase.
228
8. Transformational-Generative Grammar, including
Generative Phonology
The mainstream of linguistics in the last four decades
of the twentieth century has been shaped by the work
of Avram Noam Chomsky (b. 1928) who had ties to
Bloomfield through his teacher Zellig Harris (see Sect.
7), and to Jakobson through personal acquaintance
and through his close association with Jakobson’s
student Morris Halle (b.1923). It is widely believed
that in its concern with ‘universal’ aspects of language
Chomsky’s program is a continuation of Jakobson’s;
in any case, it had no precedent in neo-Bloomfieldian
structuralism. Such was Chomsky’s success in over-
turning mainstream structuralist tenets that it is no
exaggeration to say he revolutionized the field-
though in broader perspective even this ‘revolution’
only helped advanced linguistics further along the
path it had been following for over 150 years. Chom-
sky’s avowed aim was to bring linguistics to the level
of rigor of physics, at once the most mathematical and
the most exact of the physical sciences. In his early
years he worked directly on mathematical models of
language, but finally abandoned this line of inquiry to
devote himself to his ‘context-free phrase structure’
models of syntax and, through the 196Os, phonology
(see Erolu tion of Transformational Grammar).
8. I Chomsky ‘s ‘Standard Theory ’ (I 955-65)
Much as the search for ‘science’ led nineteenth-century
linguists to eliminate living languages and struc-
turalists to eliminate parole from their sphere of
inquiry, Chomsky too eliminated from consideration
everything but the linguistic competence of an idealized
native speaker-hearer. He utterly rejected the
behaviorism that was part of Bloomfield’s legacy,
arguing that language is not a behavioral phenomenon
but a mental attribute (later he would refer to it
as
an ‘organ’) that is ‘universal’ and ‘innate.’ (Actu-
ally, the version of linguistic behaviorism Chomsky
most memorably attacked was not Bloomfield’s, but
that of B. F. Skinner (1904-90), a prominent Harvard
‘behavioral psychologist’ not directly affiliated with
any linguistic school or tradition.)
The claim of innateness not just of general language
faculties but of specific grammatical features is what
most abruptly severs Chomsky’s thought from its predecessors,
for in certain other ways it represents a
continuity and furthering of the structuralist approach.
In terms of the features of structuralism listed
in
Sect. 5.1, only (c), the preference for social abstractions
over mental ones (which implies behaviorism in
a greater or lesser degree), is clearly abandoned by
Chomsky. The other goals are maintained or inten-
sified. Regarding methodology, Chomsky rejected distributionalism
as naively empiricist, capable only of
revealing
the trivial phenomena of ‘surface’ structure
rather than the ‘deep’ structure which constitutes a
language’s innate, universal core. Data garnered
through introspection by the linguist-formerly suspect-were
deemed superior to those acquired ‘objec-
tively’ because of the new status granted to the mind.
And the ‘transformations* which lead from deep to
surface structure became a hallmark not only of
Chomsky’s linguistics but of later structuralist
thought generally.
Where his work was in conflict with the structuralist
mainstream Chomsky sought alignment with still ear-
lier traditions, in particular with Cartesian’ linguistics
which other historians of linguistics have not followed
him in recognizing. More subtly, his representation of
phrase structure as tree diagrams tied him to a didactic
tradition that embodied much of what the structuralist
enterprise had labored against. In the early 196Os,
when the race for American superiority in space
brought unprecedented funding especially to ‘scien-
tific’ enterprises, Chomsky attracted talented disciples
from various fields and was enormously successful in
getting both publicity and government funding.
Despite the reticence or outright refusal of most
senior structuralists to accept its leading ideas, transformational-generative
grammar became mainstream
linguistics in America around the mid-1960s and was
on its way to having this status worldwide, the first
unified paradigm since neogrammarianism to do so.
Extended Standard Theory (1966-78)
Within a short time, however, dissension erupted
within the generative ranks, led by some of Chomsky’s
most talented followers (see Generative Semantics).
The central issue was Chomsky’s insistence upon the
radical autonomy of syntax. In Chomsky’s view, syn-
tactic rules represent the initial stage of language gen.
eration; phonological and semantic rules are subsequent
and ‘interpretative.’ This radical version of
the structuralist priority of form over meaning conflicted
with the common intuition that meaning per-
haps need not, but can, determine syntax to a degree
sufficient to render the ‘autonomy’ of syntax virtually
nonexistent. The colleagues who broke with Chomsky
at this time did so in the name of ‘generative’ (as
opposed to interpretative) semantics, and though this
was no organized movement with a coherent research
framework, it has had a lasting impact on the field
(see R. A. Harris 1993). Besides reviving interest in
semantics as a major subdiscipline of linguistics
(whence it was all but banished during the neo-Bloomfieldian
ascendancy), it gave rise to the thriving area
of discourse pragmatics (see Sect. 11), which studies
how topic and focus phenomena determine word
order even in supposedly ‘syntactic’ languages, and to
George Lakoff’s (b.1941) influential work on metaphor
in language and thought. Many of the leading
ideas of generative semantics were absorbed into later
breakaway generative paradigms, and soon thereafter
into Chomsky’s own program (examples include
predicate-raising, logical form, lexical decomposition,
globality; see further Sect. 8.3).
Chomsky’s response to this challenge included significant
revisions to the theory, although he continued
to
assert more strongly than ever the autonomy of
syntax and the interpretative nature of semantics. The
principal changes made in this period were a shift of
emphasis away from phrase structure rules and
toward word-specific features specified in the lexicon,
and severe restrictions placed on transformations,
which were no longer permitted to add or delete but
only to move elements already present in deep structure.
This led to a complexification of structural levels
and the introduction of ‘empty categories,’ ‘traces,’
and ‘filters* to do the work transformations formerly
did. Chomsky still insisted that grammatical roles like
subject and object must be derived from word order,
despite the success of Charles Fillmore’s (b. 1929) ‘case
grammar’ in explaining these phenomena without the
sometimes elaborate mechanisms to which Chomsky
had to resort (see Case Grammar). The result was that
by the mid-1970s the ‘extended standard theory’ had
reached a state of such complexity that its claims to
account for the speed and efficiency of child language
acquisition were growing ever poorer, and alternative
models were proliferating annually.
8.3 Government and Binding Theory: Alternative Generative
Theories
Saussurean and Jakobsonian structuralism continued
to be the dominant mode of linguistic analysis in Eur-
ope until the student revolts of 1968. Meanwhile,
scores of new universities and linguistics positions
were being created throughout Europe in the 1960s
and 1970s. The combined result was that a large post-
World War II generation of linguists starting their
careers found that the tradition in which they had
been trained was intellectually passe. Some turned to
the fashionable post-structuralist approaches sug-
gested by the work of Michel Foucault (1926-84),
Jacques Derrida (b.l930), and others. But another
group of young European linguists became Chom-
sky’s new core constituency, and it was to them, in
Pisa in 1979, that he presented his new, pared-down,
‘neoclassical’ version of generative linguistics, called
*Principles and Parameters’ or government and binding
theory (GB), the objective of which is to formulate
the ‘parameters’ of ‘core’ universal grammar in the
most general possible way. Part of the new simplicity
was gained through finally admitting a system of
grammatical relations as primary (though without
acknowledging that this represented any sort of con-
cession to rival theories), and in the final abandonment
of phrase-structure rules, with ever greater
emphasis on lexical specification. But in general, GB
shifted the focus of linguistic inquiry away from
accounting for specific problems in specific languages,
and toward relating problems within and among lan-
229
Century- Linguistics
guages. By tracing not obviously related problems to
single sources, it seemed for several years to point
toward a greater economy of explanation, and hence
toward real progress in the understanding of human
language structure.
By the late 198Os, however, new categories and prin-
ciples had begun to proliferate in the light of more
detailed data, and GB moved toward a strongly lexicalist
mode! that rejects any notion of deep or (D-)
structure. Since 1991 Chomsky himself has turned his
attention away from the kinds of problems GB was
aimed at solving, and toward a ‘minimalist program’
devoted to the investigation of features shared by a!!
languages. At present no one approach can claim
mainstream status, though it is probably accurate to
say that this status is shared by the whole cluster of
generative theories, including minimalism, GB, lexical-functional
grammar, relational grammar, and
numerous
other approaches, including generalized
phrase structure grammar (GPSG) and head-driven
phrase structure grammar (HPSG), both of which
restore phrase structure rules to the prominence they
had in earlier transformational-generative grammar,
while eliminating the transformational component
completely. (For a fuller survey of these approaches,
see Droste & Joseph 1991; see also Applicational
Grammar; Case Grammar; Cognitive Grammar.)
8.4 Generatire Phonology
Generative phonology has developed in parallel with
(and sometimes in the shadow of) syntactic theory.
Chomsky & Halle’s Sound Pattern of English (1968)
established the basis of a phonological theory that
would recapitulate the syntactic mode! in so far as
possible (an unacknowledged bond with London
School structuralism and Praguean phonology).
Because phonology, unlike syntax, consists of a fixed
number of elements, a degree of systematic com-
pleteness was achieved that syntactic studies could
never match. Early generative phonology incorporated
Jakobson’s distinctive feature theory, as we!!
as an extremely formalized version of markedness theory
(which subsequently found a prominent place in
GB as well). A less formalized version of markedness
soon gave rise to various versions of ‘natural phonology’
in which phonetic complexity interfaces with
systemic considerations to determine marked and
unmarked structures.
However, from the mid-1970s on, the mainstream
of phonological work shifted to more forma! analytical
considerations, starting with the proper rep-
resentation of ‘suprasegmentals,’ most notably stress
and the tones of tone languages (another unac-
knowledged bond with the London School). John
Goldsmith’s (b.1951) mode! of ‘autosegmental pho-
nology’ gave rise to numerous attempts at accounting
for phonology through the use of ‘tier’ and ‘template’
230
models, and of ‘underspecification’ of phonologica!
units as a way of accounting for the behavior of maxi-
mally unmarked elements.
Natural phonology led in due course to natural
morphology and syntax, which in turn gave impetus
to studies of ‘iconicity’ between sound and meaning
(another topic anticipated by Jakobson); and underspecification
theory has been extended to other levels
of linguistic structure. Even if phonology no longer
occupies the central position it did during the struc-
turalist period, it continues to spawn much original
work that exports as many ideas as it imports to other
linguistic realms.
9. Sociolinguistics
We have seen that each movement toward greater
autonomy in linguistics brings an opposite (if unequal)
reaction toward re-placing language in a more broadly
human context. Thus Humboldtian psychologism
came on the heels of early historical-comparative
linguistics, Neogrammarianism coincided with the
academic institutionalization of modern literature
studies, and structuralism arose contemporaneously
with Crocean aestheticism. So too, generative sem-
antics may be seen as a corrective reaction against the
excesses of transformationalism, as in fact may the
contemporary rise of a large number of alternative
approaches to language, three of which are surveyed
in the following sections. The most significant of these
is sociolinguistics.
The origins of linguistic geography were treated in
Sect. 3.3 above, which noted that its rise to prominence
in the early part of the twentieth century
created a momentum for synchronic linguistics.
Although the arrival of structuralism kept linguistic
geography from ever having mainstream status, it con-
tinued to be an active and vibrant research area in
Europe and, starting in the 193Os, in America, under
the leadership of Austrian-born Hans Kurath (!891-
1992). The shallower time depth of English in America
meant that geographic isog!osses were often less
clearly defined and more susceptible to socially-based
considerations. Raven McDavid (191 l-84) took up
these social factors starting in the mid-!940s, around
the same time that a group of scholars centered at
Columbia University, led by Andre Martinet (b. 1908)
and his student Urie! Weinreich (1926-67), began to
emphasize the importance of class dialects. At least
one prominent sociologist, Paul Hanly Furfey (18961992),
was training students in linguistic research as
we!!, and by the early 1950s a number of individuals
were actively engaged in collecting language data
along social-class lines. The movement did not attract
wide attention however until the early 1960s and the
early studies of William Labov (b.!927), which coincided
with increased US government interest in
funding social research and the study of black English
at the height of the civil rights movement.
Lab&s studies established themselves at once at
the forefront of sociolinguistics, to the point that earl-
ier work was largely forgotten. The reasons are by
now familiar: increased ‘scientificness,’ in particular
mathematicality, attained through a heavy reliance
on statistical information and calculation. The use of
‘variable rules’ effectively brought into the domain of
langue much that otherwise would have been relegated
to parole, providing a further systematization and
reclaiming of territory from voluntary language production.
Moreover, Labov’s demonstrations of how
social variation can be the synchronic reflex of diachronic
change has led to a significant merger between
sociolinguistics and historical linguistics in the United
States and the United Kingdom.
Sociolinguistics never achieved mainstream status
in the United States, and while very much alive there,
it has fared better still in the United Kingdom, a
country that is generally more class-conscious and has
a long tradition of dialect studies. The London School
and ‘systemic’ linguistics have always emphasized the
dual nature of language as social semiotic-and unlike
most other structural approaches, they have done
more than focus on one aspect while paying lip service
to the other.
10. Universal-Typological Linguistics
Beginning in the late 1950s Joseph H. Greenberg
(b. 1915), a linguist in the anthropological tradition,
began rethinking long-neglected questions of lan-
guage universals and typology, the latter in connection
with his important work on classifying African languages.
That his interest in universals arose sim-
ultaneously with Chomsky’s in universal grammar is,
despite their considerable differences, evidence of the
power of Jakobsonian structuralism on younger
American linguists of the period (Greenberg admits
this influence more readily than Chomsky does).
While Chomsky claimed that study of any one language
in its deep structure would by definition be a
study
of universals, Greenberg set about looking for
universals in an empirical way, by examining the
grammars of a sample of languages from numerous
language families. He found that while absolute universals,
such as having the vowel /a/, were trivial to
the deeper understanding and functioning of language
systems, a large number of ‘implicational’ universals
could be discerned that related the functioning of
seemingly disparate linguistic elements to one another
in previously unsuspected ways. For example, he
found that while languages are almost equally divided
between those which place objects before and after
verbs, and between those which place objects before
and after adpositions (a cover term for prepositions
and postpositions), the two features correlate such
.
Trends in Twentieth-century Linguistics: An Ocercie\r
that postpositional languages tend overwhelmingly to
have the order object-verb and prepositional lan-
guages to have verb-object. That is, objects tend to
come either before or after both verbs and adpositions
in a given language, suggesting that there exists a
unified process of government that supersedes that of
either of these categories.
Although Greenberg’s work was directly in line
with Chomsky’s both in the overall program of a
search for the universal and in the more specific result
of collapsing traditional distinctions into megacategories
like government, Chomsky and other gen-
erative linguists have from the beginning refused to
admit a meaningful connection between the two programs,
arguing that Greenberg dealt with mere surface
structure
phenomena and drew meaningless con-
clusions from statistical tendencies. For the neoBloomfieldians-whose
main concern had been to
avoid creating pseudo-universals by imposing the categories
and structures of their native language on the
very different languages they were investigating-and
for European structuralists generally, the empirical
nature of Greenberg’s work and that of others in the
same vein, including Bernard Comrie (b. 1947) and
John Hawkins (b. 1947), made it much more palatable
than Chomsky’s, which appeared to admit of no disproof.
Significant strides toward reconciling the
Greenbergian and Chomskyan visions of language
universals have finally been made in the last 15 years.
11. Discourse Analysis
The final trend to be considered here is remarkable
for tying together several disparate traditions in the
goal of expanding language analysis beyond the level
of the individual utterance. Traditionally this was the
goal of rhetoric, and later of ‘stylistics’ as practiced
for example by Saussure‘s associate Charles Bally. For
linguistics proper, however, the amount of (seemingly
willful) variation possible beyond the level of the
clause seemed to establish this as a firm upper limit to
the extent of langue.
This may be why the impulse to go further came
from sociology, particularly from the ‘conversational
analysis’ initiated by Harvey Sacks (1935-75) in the
196Os, within the more general paradigm of ethnomethodology
founded by Harold Garfinkel
(b. 1929).
This
work did not find common ground with Labov-
ian sociolinguistics, but did establish bonds with the
‘ethnology of speaking* approach founded by Dell H.
Hymes (b. 1927), who had been trained in the anthropological
tradition. What is more, both con-
versational analysis and the ethnography of communication
found common ground with Halliday and
the London School, as well as with Prague School
analysis of sentence perspective (see also Prague
School Syntax and Semantics). John J. Gumperz
(b. 1922) is generally credited with having drawn these
231
2Ofh Century Linguistics
various trends together in the later 1960s into the field
known as ‘discourse analysis’ (see Murray 1994).
Discourse analysis soon received valuable input
from an unlikely source: generative semantics. In mak-
ing their case against the hegemony of syntax the
generative semanticists gave particular attention to
‘pragmatics,’ the study of topic and focus phenomena
(a Praguean heritage), and pragmatics was readily
incorporated into the more general scope of discourse
analysis, which henceforth could claim probably the
richest heritage (sociological-anthropological-Genevan/American/Praguean/London
- structuralist -
generativist) of any current trend (see also Generatice
Semantics). In the early 1990s Deborah Tannen’s
(b. 1945) analyses of men’s and women’s conversational
patterns achieved unprecedented popular success
in America-and perhaps not coincidentally,
discourse analysis appears to be emerging as a contender
for mainstream status. It is drawing increasing
interest
from ‘cognitive scientists’ (see Sect. 12) as well
as
from adjacent humanistic fields, including literary
criticism.
In this convergence of research traditions we have
in a sense the final culmination of the scientificization
of language study. Over the past 200 years the dividing
line between the systematic and the willful has shifted
progressively from phonology to morphology to part
of syntax to a!! of syntax and now finally to a!! of
discourse. The result of this seems to be, in other
words, that no aspect of language need be considered
unsystematic; no aspect cannot ultimately be
accounted for in a scientific, even algebraic way.
Through all the vagaries of neogrammarianism, struc-
turalism, and generativism, behaviorism and universalism,
rationalism and empiricism, this path of
development
has continued unbroken.
12. Conclusion
For language theory the twentieth century has been a
time of great intellectual ferment, only part of which
is reflected in a chronicle of mainstreams. Much of the
remaining story is told in other, nonhistorica! entries
in the 1994 Encyclopediu of Language & Linguistics;
much more is not told, because it falls outside the
scope of linguistics as currently defined.
But the definition of the field is changing. ‘Semiotics’
has come into existence as the kind of general
science of signs envisioned by figures like Saussure
and Charles Sanders Peirce (1839-1914). Closer to
the mainstream, the rise of ‘cognitive science’ as an
umbrella category dovering linguistics as well as much
work in psychology, philosophy, computer science,
artificial intelligence, and consciousness studies, has
come about largely through the influence of Chomsky
and his school. Cognitive scientists see the auton-
omous structure of language as providing the key to
the structure of the mind; in general, they believe that
232
creating the most efficient mode! of artificial inte!ligence
is the best possible way of discovering how the
mind operates. This notion has however come into
conflict with another view of the mind and its oper-
ation, called ‘connectionism,’ which is skeptical about
the claims of cognitive science and prefers to construct
explanations through a combination of evidence
acquired through direct study of neural networks and
processes of perception and mental organization as
well as of language structure. The challenge for
linguistics as the twentieth century ends is to reassert
the autonomous status of language and account for
its interaction with other, equally autonomous mental
faculties.
It has become a commonplace to associate the rise
of formalism and structuralism in late nineteenth- and
twentieth-century thought with the rise of Marxism in
both the intellectual and the political spheres. If the
connection is indeed real, then we should not be sur-
prised if the demise of Marxism portends radical
change in Western thought, including linguistics. We
should not even be surprised if the 200-year course
of scientificization described at the beginning of this
article were, finally, reversed. A revival of interest in
problematizing the ‘autonomy’ of linguistics, nascent
at the time of this writing, may prove to be the first
tentative step in this direction.
Bibliography
Anderson S R 1985 Phonology in the 20th Centuw: Theories
of Rules and. Theories of Representation. University of
Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois
Andresen J T 1990 Linguistics in America 1769-1924. Rout-
ledge, London
Botha R P 1992 Twentieth Century Conceptions of Language.
Blackwell, Oxford
Darnell R 1990 Edward Sapir: Linguist, Anthropologist,
Humanist. University of California Press, Berkeley, California
Droste F G, Joseph J E feds.) 1991 Linguistic Theory and
Grammatical Description. Benjamins, Amsterdam
Figueroa E 1994 Sociolinguistic Metatheory. Pergamon,
Oxford
Harris R (ed.) 1988 Linguistic Thought in England, 1914-45.
Duckworth, London
Harris R A 1993 The Linguistics Wars. Oxford University
Press, New York
Hymes D, Fought J 1981 American Structuralism. Mouton.
The Hague
Joseph J E, Taylor T J 1990 Ideologies of Language. Rout-
ledge, London
Koerner E F K 1973 Ferdinand de Saussure: Origin and
Decelopment of his Linguistic Thought in Western Studies
of Language. Vieweg, Braunschweig
Matthews P H I993 Grammatical Theory in the United States:
From Bloomfield to Chomsky. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge
Murray S 0 I994 Theory Groups and the Study of Language in
North America: A Sociul History. Benjamins, Amsterdam
Saussurean Tradition in Linguistics
Newmeyer F J 1986 Linguistic ‘Theoy in Anlericu, 2nd edn.
Steiner P (ed.) 1982 The Prague School: Selected
Academic Press, Orlando, Florida
Writings 192946. University of Texas Press, Austin,
pedersen H 193 I The Discoceys qf’ Lunguuge. Harvard Uni- Texas
versity Press. Cambridge, Massachusetts
Tagliavini C 1968 Punoranta di storia della linguistica, 2nd
Robins R H 1990 A Short History* qf Linguistics, 3rd edn. edn. R Patron, Bologna
Longman. London
Vachek J 1966 The ‘Linguistic School of Prague: An Intro-
Sampson G 1980 Schools qf Linguistics: Competition und
duction to its Theory and Practice. Indiana University
Erolution. Hutchinson, London
Press, Bloomington, Indiana
No comments:
Post a Comment